Why a Diagram is (Sometimes) Worth Ten Thousand Words

Why a Diagram is (Sometimes) Worth Ten Thousand Words

11, 65-99 (1987) | JILL H. LARKIN, HERBERT A. SIMON
The article by Jill H. Larkin and Herbert A. Simon from Carnegie-Mellon University explores the advantages of diagrammatic representations over sentential representations in problem-solving. They distinguish between two types of information-processing systems: sentential and diagrammatic. Sentential representations are sequential, like propositions in a text, while diagrammatic representations are indexed by location in a plane, often displaying implicit information explicitly. The authors argue that the computational efficiency of these representations depends on the information-processing operators that act on them. Diagrammatic representations can be more efficient because they organize information by location, allowing for smoother problem-solving through direct access to relevant information. In contrast, sentential representations require extensive search and computation to explicitize implicit information. The article provides examples from physics and geometry to illustrate the differences. For instance, in a pulley problem, a diagrammatic representation significantly reduces the number of searches needed compared to a sentential representation. Similarly, in a geometry problem involving congruent triangles, a diagrammatic representation facilitates recognition of key elements, reducing the need for labeling and enhancing problem-solving efficiency. The authors conclude that the value of diagrams lies in their ability to make problem-solving more intuitive and efficient, particularly in domains where visual information is crucial, such as physics and engineering. They emphasize that the effectiveness of diagrams depends on the availability of appropriate inference rules and the nature of the problem being solved.The article by Jill H. Larkin and Herbert A. Simon from Carnegie-Mellon University explores the advantages of diagrammatic representations over sentential representations in problem-solving. They distinguish between two types of information-processing systems: sentential and diagrammatic. Sentential representations are sequential, like propositions in a text, while diagrammatic representations are indexed by location in a plane, often displaying implicit information explicitly. The authors argue that the computational efficiency of these representations depends on the information-processing operators that act on them. Diagrammatic representations can be more efficient because they organize information by location, allowing for smoother problem-solving through direct access to relevant information. In contrast, sentential representations require extensive search and computation to explicitize implicit information. The article provides examples from physics and geometry to illustrate the differences. For instance, in a pulley problem, a diagrammatic representation significantly reduces the number of searches needed compared to a sentential representation. Similarly, in a geometry problem involving congruent triangles, a diagrammatic representation facilitates recognition of key elements, reducing the need for labeling and enhancing problem-solving efficiency. The authors conclude that the value of diagrams lies in their ability to make problem-solving more intuitive and efficient, particularly in domains where visual information is crucial, such as physics and engineering. They emphasize that the effectiveness of diagrams depends on the availability of appropriate inference rules and the nature of the problem being solved.
Reach us at info@study.space
Understanding Why a Diagram is (Sometimes) Worth Ten Thousand Words